StudentShare
Contact Us
Sign In / Sign Up for FREE
Search
Go to advanced search...
Free

Good Taste and Good Sense Models - Essay Example

Cite this document
Summary
This essay "Good Taste and Good Sense Models" focuses on the revival of sexual selection theory that came under the guidance of researchers in theoretical population genetics, primatology, evolutionary psychology, experimental, and behavioural biology.  …
Download full paper File format: .doc, available for editing
GRAB THE BEST PAPER93.2% of users find it useful
Good Taste and Good Sense Models
Read Text Preview

Extract of sample "Good Taste and Good Sense Models"

? Good Taste and Good Sense Models The employment of sexual selection theory to human behaviour is a prominent topic in evolutionary psychology and forms one of the most productive and compelling developments within human sciences in the last two decades. Paradoxically, this development may have been perceived as ridiculous only two decades ago. Majority of biologists at that time perceived sexual selection via mate choice to be Darwin’s least appealing idea, if not utterly wrong. At that time, any “darwinization” of human sciences would have been grounded in natural selection theory, which does not possess a significant impact on human, sexual, social, and cultural behaviour. Good Taste and Good Sense Models Introduction Mate choice infers the behavioural manifestation of mate preferences. These preferences are mainly “mental adaptations” executed as multifaceted neural circuits, and constructed via the interaction of many genes and environmental conditions, which may prejudice mating in support of individuals with definite perceivable traits. In many species, such systems might function without conscious responsiveness, consideration, or intricate feelings; however, one might expect mate choice to be one of the least unconscious of an animal’s decisions as it demands the integration of diverse information (Cartwright, 2000). Mate choice operates through rejecting some possible mates and tolerating or seeking others. In a majority of species, females can successfully resist copulations attempts by unwanted males, and in a number of cases, females aggressively solicit copulations from desired males. Similarly, males usually pursue desired females, and disregard solicitation attempts from redundant females (Gallagher, Nelson & Weiner, 2003). In most instances, mutual choice and cooperation are essential for breeding. The mechanism for mate choice evolve continuously; being choosy demands time, energy, and intelligence whereby these costs of mate choice bear the capability to harm the survival and can decrease the probability of sexual selection operating effectively. It is rewarding to be choosy since, in sexually reproducing species, the genetic worth of one species determines half the genetic composition of the offspring (Cartwright, 2000). The formation of a dual genetic endeavour with an attractive, high, superior mate enhances the chances of transfer of high quality genes. Mate choice can be regarded to be the best eugenics and genetic screening that female are competent to carry out under field conditions devoid of using any equipment, but their senses and brains (Anderson, 1994). Mate choice mechanisms can expand via direct selection for mate choices efficiency whereby improved preferences yield better/more offspring. Similarly, mate choice mechanisms can also develop via other less conventional, less adaptive processes such as genetic drift, mutation, and genetic linkage with other traits that may be experiencing genetic drift, natural selection, or sexual selection (Andersson, 1994). Apart from the first processes, the last three processes characteristically yield harmful changes in mate choice mechanism, and hence are mainly selected out. Nevertheless, some of the changes endure via chance, utility, or Fisher’s runaway effect. The irregularity of the three processes is essential in illuminating the diversity of sexually-selected ornaments across matching, closely-linked species. One of the simplest means to review the present state of sexual selection theory is to investigate the diverse criteria that animals employ to choose mates. This is informed by the fact that one can often perceive sexual competition within each sex as a product of mate choice by the other choice, whereby choice in this case encompass processes that are both mindful and unconscious, psychological or physiological (Gallagher, Nelson & Weiner, 2003). Sexual selection can be divided into two leading approaches; natural selection and sexual selection. Natural selection (good sense) infers differential survival, while sexual selection (good taste) encompasses differential reproduction. Good taste Approach Charles Darwin established the foundation for all contemporary work on sexual selection as outlined in his influential book, The Descent of Man. In his analysis, Darwin set out the mechanism of sexual selection, which shapes one of the hypotheses that he had outlined in the Origin of Species. Natural and sexual selection make up the twin pillars of Darwin’s adaptationist paradigm (Iwasa, Pomiankowski & Nee, 1991). However, one should be careful not to overstate the dissimilarity between the two approaches of selection. Whether one survives to reproduce or avoid predators, or whether one is triumphant in attracting mates, the equivalent principle of the disparity survival of genes is at play. Certainly, some of the features that aid in circumventing predators may also be employed in securing a mate. The other notion of his theory featured aggressive competition manifested between males for mates. The apparent competition was perceived to be noncontentious since it appeared to call for characteristics such as muscles, a quick response, and claws, all of which natural selection will approve of, all the same. Darwin’s contentious idea of female choice attracted a lot of interest (Iwasa, Pomiankowski & Nee, 1991). According to Darwin, females frequently choose their mates exclusively based on aesthetic grounds. Good taste model also referred to as “aesthetic” (as Darwin proposed) gives females its selective advantage with taste alone being the driving force. He noted “a significant number of males had been made beautiful solely for beauty’s sake, and that the most purified beauty may act as a fascination for the female, devoid of any other purpose.” The Darwinian sexual selection advances that females possess an integrated aesthetic for a certain trait or form of trait manifested among males. Darwin theory of sexual selection (1874) attempted to elucidate the evolution of characters such as tail plumes of the peacock, antlers of deer, and radiant colouration of a variety of birds and their incredible exhibits and songs, as outlined by the collective effect of females’ preference for distinct male types (Iwasa, Pomiankowski & Nee, 1991). Darwin observed that Peahens tend to prefer mating with the most attractive and plentifully tailed males. The long tails will thus be represented in the next generation, and vice versa. Darwin noted that though evolutionary time, females frequently exert an ever-demanding, persistent pressure, with their mates countering with ever-enhanced grandeur until the peacock’s finery (extravagant male display) rises to the exuberance manifested today (Crawford & Krebs, 1998). Darwin proposed that the drawbacks to male survival occasioned by such personalities are recompensed by improved females desiring a certain individual to other males. Nevertheless, Darwin could not adequately explain why females should desire definite males. The theory of sexual selection stimulated and still elicits much debate (Crawford & Krebs, 1998). There is always a basic complexity to be explained. On one hand, it is a principal observation that the majority of attractive bird species (male birds), or deer manifesting enormous antlers, are universally favoured by females; on the other hand, there is an absence of a straightforward explanation implying ways that make some males be of better quality than others. The other Darwinians were not impressed by the “good taste” theory of female choice. The then critiques claimed that Darwin had persistently given too much weight to the notion that bright colour, plus other prominent characters must possess a sexual function. Wallace (1889), thus, dismissed overall the theory of sexual selection by mate preference, while others like Poulton (1890), supported it. Although, the Darwinians in this tradition did not entirely rule out sexual selection, they allocated, at best, an inadequate role. Empirical investigations conducted over a broad range of species highlight that females are frequently sensible (Crawford & Krebs, 1998). R.A. Fisher’s (1930) Runaway Theory The significance for indicators in sexual selection has been highlighted by R.A. Fisher (1915), Zahavi (1975), and Williams (1966). Indicators such as animal signals are mainly subject to the handicap principle and evolve effortlessly when they are “condition-dependent,” whereby healthier animals adopt better care or guarantee use of the indicators they possess. According to R.A. Fisher’s (1930) runaway theory, runaway is maintained by the “momentum” bestowed upon the genetic linkage and risk to persons of failing to display exaggerated traits or choosy preferences emanating from the momentum (Crawford & Krebs, 1998). Fisher’s model of sexual selection stipulates that males possess costly ornaments, of which females choose between males based on those ornaments, and incur costs so as to attain that end. This approach stipulate that initial female preference becomes self-reinforcing whereby a runaway effect created yields elaborate and frequently dysfunctional (in regard to natural selection) appendages, as is the case of peacock’s train. One of the other two processes that also account for the evolution of sexually selected traits encompasses runaway sexual selection (Crawford & Krebs, 1998). Fisher (1930) advocated that originally there was a connection between the preferred personality by the female and the superiority of the male. The approach stipulates that the preferences and traits co-evolve via a feedback process that can yield rapid evolution of certain traits for fundamentally arbitrary reasons. This association, when realized by discriminating females, can contribute for the preliminary attractiveness of the male. The onset of sexual selection generates a quick chain of events, whereby the preference itself generates a selective pressure accounting for the amplified development of personality, and its subsequent attractiveness (Gallagher, Nelson & Weiner, 2003). According to Fisher, males possess inflated sexually selected characters that are more appealing to females, not essentially because the subjects are better now than they were before, but primarily because the males plus their sons are more attractive to females. Runaway is significantly forceful and pervasive force that arises even in genetic models of indicators; however, runaway is also significantly stochastic process, which is also sensitive to preliminary conditions and hence capable of elaborating the impulsive divergence of sexual ornamentation registered across species. The three fundamental assumptions of Fisher’s model have significantly been reinforced by recent empirical work, namely: 1) persons with substantial sexual ornaments usually manifest better mating success, although, with lower survival compared with those with smaller ornaments; 2) the relevant traits and preferences indicate inherited genetic variation; 3) there is also genetic association between the pertinent traits and preferences (Gallagher, Nelson & Weiner, 2003). The runaway process is also reinforced by findings that a variety of animals copy each other’s mate choices in a way demonstrating that they are following an arbitrary fashion, instead of a reliable indicator. Majority of Darwin’s followers critique Fisher’s approach by asserting that his approach does not adequately clarify the basis, and adaptive purpose of female choice. Supporters of R. A. Fisher’s (1930) runaway process have underlined the choice for aesthetic displays, also referred to as “good taste” or “sexy son” selection. In evolutionary biology, the diverse mate choice principles are predominantly considered as competing models on how sexual selection works (Gallagher, Nelson & Weiner, 2003). Fisher’s runway selection idea outlines that the trait being selected does not strictly minimize fitness, although under it does when under altered conditions. Good Genes sexual selection theory (Good sense theory) Good genes sexual selection theory forms a part of the alternative (although frequently compatible) middle-level theories of sexual selection. Good sense (genes) approach features the female employing signals from the male to signify the resistance of the male to parasites. Good sense (resources) details that females inspect resources held by males, and explore the willingness of males to commit resources (potential for altruistic behaviour). This may demonstrate the potential of genetic quality. The fundamental idea of good sense sexual selection approach infers that the outcome of mate choice and intersexual competition will be shaped by traits that show high genetic viability (Anderson, 1994). For instance, males (and to a lesser degree, females) of a variety of bird species, possess a perplexing variety of ornaments such as elongated tail feathers. Maintaining such elaborate visual ornamentation in excellent condition is not essentially an easy task as it demands time, good health, and effort. Females, who constantly opt for the brightest, most ornamented males, are highly probable to be selecting males who are in the best condition. This reflects a constant pursuit for males with underlying genetic quality. Irrespective of whether the females receive nothing extra apart from sperm from their mates, the females will most likely have healthier, improved, and highly attractive offspring in the event that the females mate with the best quality males. As indicated, the bulk of secondary sexual characteristics hence operate as pointers of genetic quality. Furthermore, according to the handicap principle advanced by Amotz Zahavi (1975; Zahavi & Zahavi, 1997), traits should be costly to produce provided that they are to function as reliable indicators of genetic worth. In instances where a trait is expensive to produce, the trait cannot act as a reference for good sense sexual selection since it will not precisely reflect the characteristics of its owner (Zahavi, 1975). Nevertheless, in instances in which the trait depends on an extensive investment of metabolic resources to establish, such as in the case of male widowbird’s tail, only individuals enjoying the best condition will be capable of developing the largest or brightest ornament (Zahavi, 1975). Hence, the expression of the trait perfectly manifests the underlying condition. Trivers (1972) outlined why males court and females choose, whereby he asserted that higher levels of essential “parental investment” by females of the majority of species transform females to be a limiting resource over which males must compete. Hence, sex differences orchestrated by parental investment propel sex differences in the intensity of sexual selection. Trivers asserts that the sex that devotes the least will compete with the sex that empowers the most, of which the sex that invests the most will have a lot to miss in the occasion of a poor match (Trivers, 1972). In a slightly divergent expression of the handicap principle, Simpson and Gangestad (1992) assert that, in males, significant levels of testosterone, which are critical to the successful expression of secondary sexual characteristics (sex linked traits that results from sexual selection), also bear detrimental effects on the immune system. According to this approach (immune competence handicap model), only the best males manifesting healthy secondary sexual characteristics that perfectly point to a high level of testosterone and a robust immune system, and subsequently an enhanced genetic quality, attract the best females. These assertions have been extensively been supported by meta-analysis of studies centering on parasite-mediated sexual selection (Anderson, 1994). Majority of the studies conducted on this model demonstrate a powerful negative association between parasite load and the exhibition of male secondary sexual characteristics. Overall, the most generously ornamented individuals also appear to be the healthiest, thus the most preferred as mates. Similarly, in species that manifest considerable parental investment (inclusive of humans), males also indicate preferences to be choosy concerning whom they mate with, and will highly prefer mates exhibiting high genetic fitness. In a variety of bird species, for instance, females and males manifest brightly coloured feathers, and may engage in complex courtship dances, which translate to a relative degree of parental investment by males and females that may significantly influence the mechanics of good sense sexual selection (Anderson, 1994). Genes in this case are not the only essentials that are exchanged from a mate to the other among sexually active species. Even though the male long-tailed widowbird contributes nothing more than sperm to prospect offspring, parental investment in diverse species by both sexes can be considerable. Parental investment is essential as it benefits both sexes in attending to a range of resources that mate donates to subsequent offspring. As a result, the express phenotypic benefits that emanates from mates and mating shapes one of the dynamic forces behind sexual selection (Cartwright, 2000). Humans are known to provide long-term pair-bonding and bi parental rearing of the offspring. Hence, on top of the traits that indicate the presence of abundant genes, the sexes (male and female) should be wary of characteristics that manifest the ability and eagerness of potential mates to dedicate time and external resources to future offspring. This assertion has been demonstrated in diverse studies on human mate preferences whereby both males and females elevate kindness and warmth, as the most significant attributes to be found in long-term mates. Partners manifesting personality traits of honesty, kindness, and warmth in someone are highly likely to remain in lasting relationships, as they are likely to invest time and resources in prospective offspring (Cartwright, 2000). Women (more so than men) also charge the incidence of status and resource-accruing potential as significant attributes in prospective mates, which is suggestive that males with the capability to contribute external resources to future offspring are better placed and favoured. It is essential to recognize that some characteristics may herald both finest genes and the capability to avail direct phenotypic benefits; hence, the two approaches of sexual selection are not strictly incompatible. For instance, a male bird manifesting bright, polished plumage may be favoured as a mate, not simply because of the apparent high genetic quality, but also because of the mate is less probable to transmit parasites to future sexual partners (Simpson & Gangestad, 1992). Nevertheless, the compatibility between good taste and good sense is frequently not evident, and it is proposed that the comparative significance of the two sexual selection criteria will differ on a species-by-species basis. Similarly, differentiation may occur within species in the comparative weighting of good sense against good taste in mate selection. For instance, Gnagestad and Simpson (2000) assert that human females frequently execute trade-offs between males possessing traits that signal enhanced probability of paternal investment. Some women in some instances pursue reasonably unrestricted strategy of engaging in short lasting sexual relationships with partners high in genetic quality, whereas others may assume a restricted strategy of pursuing long-term partners who are likely to avail considerable paternal investment (Crawford & Krebs, 1998). Contrary to good sense sexual selection, empirical evidence for the runaway process is comparatively thin. A core aspect of the runaway approach is that the preferred traits are arbitrary connected to fitness in the logic that such traits fail to indicate genetic viability (as is often the case of superior gene models). Nevertheless, the runaway process starts with a preference that is mainly grounded in real viability. The sensory bias model of sexual selection, on the other hand, encompasses the evolution of traits through sexual selection owing to preferences emanating from sensory orientations that are resultant of other selective processes (Cartwright, 2000). Contrary to good sense model and runaway sexual selection model, where preferences and traits mainly coevolve, in the sensory bias model, demands the initial origin of preferences and the subsequent evolution of traits that utilize such preferences. Conclusion The revival of sexual selection theory over the last two decades came under the guidance of researchers in theoretical population genetics, primatology, evolutionary psychology, and experimental, behavioural biology. Presently, although natural selection theory forms the bedrock of evolutionary psychology (conceptual and rhetorical foundation), sexual selection theory has emerged strongly and shapes the day-to-day research. Sexual selection has evolved to turn into one of the emerging and most thrilling topics within the arena of evolutionary biology and animal behaviour. Good genes (sense) model of sexual selection has evolved to become a critical middle level theory that is essential in generating several fascinating and testable hypotheses relating to both human and non-human behaviour. In most cases, the extent of choosiness that an individual manifests in selecting a partner is closely associated with the level of commitment and investment demonstrated by either party. References Anderson, M. (1994). Sexual selection. New Jersey, NJ: Princeton University Press. Cartwright, J. (2000). Evolution and human behaviour. Bradford, UK: Bradford Books. Crawford, C. & Krebs, D. (1998). Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications. New Jersey, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Gallagher, M., Nelson, R. & Weiner, I. (2003). Handbook of Psychology: Biological Psychology. New Jersey, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. Iwasa, Y., Pomiankowski, A., & Nee, S. (1991). The evolution of costly mate preferences: The ‘handicap’ principle. Evolution 4.5(6), 1431-1442. Simpson, J. A., & Gangestad, S. W. (1992). Sociosexuality and romantic partner choice. Journal of personality 60, 31-52. Trivers, R. (1972). Social evolution. California, CA: Benjamin/Cummings. Zahavi, A. (1975). Mate selection: A selection for a Handicap. Journal of Theoretical Biology 53, 205-214. Read More
Cite this document
  • APA
  • MLA
  • CHICAGO
(“Are the good taste and good sense models of sexual selection Essay”, n.d.)
Retrieved from https://studentshare.org/psychology/1401862-are-the-good-taste-and-good-sense-models-of-sexual
(Are the Good Taste and Good Sense Models of Sexual Selection Essay)
https://studentshare.org/psychology/1401862-are-the-good-taste-and-good-sense-models-of-sexual.
“Are the Good Taste and Good Sense Models of Sexual Selection Essay”, n.d. https://studentshare.org/psychology/1401862-are-the-good-taste-and-good-sense-models-of-sexual.
  • Cited: 0 times

CHECK THESE SAMPLES OF Good Taste and Good Sense Models

Analysis of the Talent Pools at Amazon and Selection

Weaknesses The company is mainly present in developed parts of the world where some developing countries are not fully covered The payment methods used by Amazon are not compatible with other countries' banking sectors Organization's external environmental factors Opportunities The company has opportunities to partner with different retailers which will help it to increase the revenue it generates.... selection of pivotal talent pool 5 Part 3: 3.... Analysis of the talent pools at Amazon and selection 7 3....
14 Pages (3500 words) Research Paper

Recent Developments in the use of Titanium Alloys as Implant Materials

Titanium alloys are chemically and biologically more compatible with human fluids and tissues but effects of body fluids and metal sensitivity may cause some problems with these implants.... Titanium and its alloys are widely used in surgical implants due to their corrosion resistance, tissue-compatibility and low density....
11 Pages (2750 words) Essay

Incident Response Plan in Case of Fire Hazards

It must also be ensured that automated endoscope preprocessors are compatible with the disinfecting agents employed.... It's a state of the art fully equipped hospital.... It has around 25 doors and 15 emergencies exist.... Burglar and fire alarms are located at every floor and at a point around 75 people are expected. Patients are the victims of CT… CT makes up for the most diagnostic radiation exposure to all patients....
18 Pages (4500 words) Essay

Utopia and liberated Women in Contemporary Advertising

This drive according to Fowles (1996) has been a necessary path taken due to the persistent… Within this study, the result of this selection has popularised the media culture in advertisement and led to a jaded audience of the same.... The central attempt of all advertisers by all costs is to convince the focus group to see the product that they are advertising as the ideal or ultimate product above all other products....
16 Pages (4000 words) Essay

Understanding Consciousness - Is Materialism Compatible with Qualia

This paper "Understanding Consciousness - Is Materialism compatible with Qualia?... This paper, then, will explore the concepts of consciousness beginning with a current understanding of the term and the basic ideas involved in materialism before investigating the concept of qualia to determine whether materialism and qualia are compatible....
16 Pages (4000 words) Essay

Wal Marts Midlife Crisis

al-Mart protects the rights of the investor by promoting a 'feel-good' factor through intense public relations exercises.... The author of the present coursework "Wal Marts Midlife Crisis" comments on how with $245 billion in revenues, Wal-mart is the biggest company in the US.... As the text has it, Wal-mart exports from China accounted for one-tenth of the US exports … Yet marked by slow growth and a change in customer preferences the ground beneath Wal-Mart is slipping and other small organizations with slick business machinery are rising fast....
25 Pages (6250 words) Coursework

Science vs Religion - an Impetus for Changing the Habitual Worldview

The dichotomy has pitted mostly scientists against religious scholars but the current trend shows there are various attempts to reconcile these two contrasting views and even religion advocates have accepted evolution as not paradigms incompatible with the most basic religious beliefs....
25 Pages (6250 words) Coursework

Infantile and Cuteness in Japanese Contemporary Art

… ABSTRACT Infantile and cuteness in Japanese contemporary art provides a unique look at Japanese culture today.... The rise of cuteness in Japanese culture emerged in the year 1970's as new style of writing after which many teenage girls later on adopted ABSTRACT Infantile and cuteness in Japanese contemporary art provides a unique look at Japanese culture today....
29 Pages (7250 words) Thesis
sponsored ads
We use cookies to create the best experience for you. Keep on browsing if you are OK with that, or find out how to manage cookies.
Contact Us