StudentShare
Contact Us
Sign In / Sign Up for FREE
Search
Go to advanced search...
Free

Perception of Cause and Behavior towards an Illness - Essay Example

Cite this document
Summary
The paper "Perception of Cause and Behavior towards an Illness" states that a sense of control such as that experienced by internals led to improved emotional and mental health, reduced risk of heart disease, better self-rated health status, and lower morbidity…
Download full paper File format: .doc, available for editing
GRAB THE BEST PAPER91.4% of users find it useful
Perception of Cause and Behavior towards an Illness
Read Text Preview

Extract of sample "Perception of Cause and Behavior towards an Illness"

Introduction Physical symptoms often are ambiguous and open for ive interpretation. Given the quantity of information that is available toindividuals at any given time, it is functional for the organism to organize and reduce the incoming data. Cognitively structuring, synthesizing, and organizing the incoming information achieve this. Cognitive psychology has introduced several theoretical constructs in this context, namely schemata, cognitive sets, prototypes, expectancies, and scripts. Studies on the structure of illness schemata consistently found five dimensions according to which symptom and illness experiences are organized (Lau and Hartman 1983): Illness schemata include information about the identity or label of the disease, its consequences (i.e., the symptoms), its causes, its duration, appropriate treatments, and expectations about its cures. Cognitive structures such as illness schemata helps to organize information from internal sensations (symptoms) and disease-related information gathered from the external environment. Howard Leventhal and his colleagues (Leventhal et al. 1984) have proposed the most relevant model of symptom perception within the tradition of information processing. Their Common Sense Models of Illness approach is rooted in cognitive psychology and builds on the work on cognitive schemata and prototypes. In specific, Leventhal et al. propose that individuals tend to construct their own individual representation of symptoms or illness and that this idiosyncratic representation will, in turn, influence their behavior (e.g., help-seeking, adherence and compliance). In specific, they proposed that illness representations (or schemata) are a function of an individual's semantic knowledge about symptoms and disease and specific contextual factors such as the nature of somatic changes and the situations in which these occur. This semantic knowledge accumulates across the life span and is acquired through the media, through personal experience, and from family and friends who have had experience with the disease. Therefore, the perception of cause can have an effect on the behavior towards an illness, because the cognitive dissemination leads to the semantic knowledge of the disease. Understanding how a person views a disease can impact behavior towards the illness. People's common sense models of illness strongly influence which symptoms a person will search for and will ultimately perceive. Work by Meyer and his colleagues (Meyer et al. 1985) on hypertension illustrates this point. Hypertension disease holds one or a combination of disease models about high blood pressure. As Meyer and his colleagues demonstrated, some patients hold the belief that hypertension is a disease of the heart, others believe that it is an arterial disorder and a third group might associate hypertension with emotional upset. Importantly, the kind of belief patients' hold affects the way they monitor their body: Symptoms that are consistent with their specific illness belief are more likely to get noticed. The relationship between a person's cognitive knowledge of the cause is therefore directly related towards the symptoms and evaluation of 'cures' for the illness. This research study will incorporate that foundation of illness cause perception by quantitatively examining the differences between illness cause perception amongst participants. In addition to general concepts of illness, people also hold organized conceptions-termed disease prototypes-for particular diseases (Bishop 1991; Bishop and Converse 1986). Prototypes of specific diseases help people organize and evaluate information about bodily sensations that might otherwise not be interpretable. Thus, a person who holds the belief that he or she is vulnerable to heart disease is more likely to interpret chest pain in accord to his or her prototype of heart disease than a person who does not hold this belief (Bishop and Converse 1986). This latter person might instead regard the chest pain as signaling a gall bladder problem. Altogether, organized representations of illness-related knowledge and experiences strongly determine symptom awareness and interpretation, and the expectation individuals generate respecting their consequences and causes. This has important implications for health behavior as the kind of beliefs patients hold about symptoms and disease may lead them to alter or fail to adhere to their treatment regimens. Purpose of the Study There is a correlation between perception of cause and behavior towards an illness. The purpose of the study is to examine this relationship, based on theoretical evidence described above. The approach of this research design is varied from other research in that the scenarios where given with or without the illness label, why is the label so important and will it make any difference to the attribution of cause. This, unlike previous research, allows an interpretation that is not biased by stating the illness. Comparatively, this defines attribution beyond a specific illness such as hypertension noted in the previous research. The aim of this research is to investigate why different individuals judge the same illness as being attributable to either a physical or psychological cause. Specifically, is there a relationship between the individuals' locus of control and the judgement they make of the causal attribute of another persons illness, as according to Weinman, Wright and Johnston (1995) control beliefs are based on causal attributions. The illnesses to be used to determine if there is such a relationship are the Functional Somatic Syndromes of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS), Late Whiplash (LW) and Gulf War Syndrome (GWS). These syndromes have been selected as there is incomplete knowledge regarding the etiologic roles of biological, psychological and sociocultural factors within the syndromes themselves (Barsky & Borus, 1999), the symptoms are also medically unexplained (Wessely, Nimnuan, Sharpe, 1999). Chronic Fatigue Syndrome Chronic fatigue syndrome is a variant illness entity that is made up of a mysterious combination of parts, which defy not only a clear description, but also more exacting calculations as to how the complex illness is created. A review of the breadth of the medical and clinical literature over the past decade alone reveals a staggering quest for explanation Jason et al 1999; Jason et al 2000. Komaroff 2000). Currently the two major medical explanations of the aetiology of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome are pathologies of the central nervous system and immune system dysregulation (McCully et al 1996; Komaroff 2000), others suggest that CFS results from somatization of psychological symptoms (Lipowski 1988; Wessely et al 1998), with three psychiatric models of the causes - the depression model, the somatization model and the stress model (Farrar et al 1995). Research suggests The shift towards favour of psychiatric models for Chronic Fatigue Syndrome is an indication that biomedicine has lost confidence in finding a purely biological agent of disease causation for Chronic Fatigue Syndrome. Ware (1992) argues that the configuration of symptoms of CFS has been constructed and reconstructed into new forms, identified and then discussed at length in new terms in popular and professional discourse over time. Sociologists of health and illness and sociology of the body (Shilling 1993;Turner 1996) go a step further and argue in essence that the medical and lay experts have become embattled for control of the definitions, practices and meanings associated with medicine. Therefore, Chronic Fatigue Syndrome was chosen as an illness variable for the hypothesis because of the recognized 'possible' causes as biological and psychological. Late Whiplash Syndrome Whiplash injuries occur in more than 1 million people in the United States every year. A small minority complains of symptoms that persist for years, often with no clear demonstrable cause. Most modern work recognises that the late whiplash syndrome is not the result of a chronic injury, and current opinion implicates a biological and psychosocial model. Scholars have explored that chronic whiplash and minor head-injury litigants produced more invalid signs than severely injured persons on measures of simulated memory and motor deficits irrespective of definitional stringency (Greiffenstein and Baker 2006). These findings support Miller and Cartlidge's (1972) observations that embellishment rises as injury severity decreases, thus validating the psychological component of Late Whiplash Syndrome (Greiffenstein and Baker 2006). However, no systematic analysis of post-traumatic pain development covering several weeks has yet been performed in whiplash patients, and the dynamics of pressure pain due to whiplash injury can be quantified by means of PC-interactive pressure algesimetry, exemplifying the biological cause of chronic whiplash pain (Nebel et al 2005). Other researchers examined that Late Whiplash Syndrome can not be supported by physical and biological evidence and that chronic symptoms were not usually caused by the car accident, but rather the socio-psychological schema of the injury (Schrader et al 1996). Therefore, because there are multi-variant causes of Late Whiplash Syndrome, the chronic illness was also chosen as a variable in this research study. Gulf War Syndrome Gulf War Syndrome is a medical condition of uncertain origin that has affected many veterans of the 1991 Persian Gulf War. Fatigue, headaches, dizziness, nausea, rashes, joint pain and respiratory problems characterize it. Since the end of the 1991 Gulf War about 20,000 United States veterans and similar proportions of troops from other allied contingents have been affected by a variety of symptoms which have collectively become known as 'Gulf War Syndrome'. Similar symptoms have been reported in Iraqi civilians including children. Despite extensive investigations no agreement has been reached on whether there is an underlying cause or causes (Emmerova and Jirava 2004). Some scholars examine the causes of exposure to the following somatic causes have been suggested as possible explanations for these symptoms: (persistent) infection, abnormal immune response, administration of multiple vaccinations within a short period of time, use of malaria chemoprophylaxis, neurological abnormalities, exposure to toxicological substances and environmental factors. (Soetekouw et al 2000). Others describe the psychological causes as exposure to traumatic events during the war, suggesting reactions occurred with a frequency were seen most in individuals exposed to a greater number, and to more severe types, of traumatic events (Kingham, Nicolaou and Southwick 1998). The exposure factor has a common bond of being active military during the Gulf War. Symptoms and causes seem to vary between biological exposure and psychological stress, prompting this chronic illness as being one of contest within the medical field. Significance of the Study The research centers on addressing cause of an illness. The cause of an illness is important because, as noted in previous, causation affects behavior. Weiner (1998) notes that illnesses diagnosed to be attributing to physical cases involves a condition with onset beyond the person's control; therefore, evoking compassion and concern for the afflicted. On the other hand, psychological disorders tend to be viewed as controllable and reversible. The condition does not bring out compassion - and may even elicit anger or indifference to the patient. With this, illness explanations must consider the patient's concerns, particularly the implication of a diagnostic label. Kirmayer, et al (2004) insist that despite inherent limitations to medical explanations, clinicians and patients are anticipating that every troubling symptom will be have adequate explanations and corresponding treatments. Successful clinical interventions that prove to be effective in eliminating symptoms will no longer need clear explanations, and may be considered relatively unimportant. However, in cases of failed treatments and persisting symptoms, the role of explanations may offer a significant amount of reassurance and explanation. This mechanism will also assist in the coping and resilience of the patient, therefore making the patient able to do a more realistic and achievable plan on the state of health in the future. The assumption of illness in a person usually leads to self-scrutiny, prompting confirmatory searches that lead into the interpretations of ambiguous sensations previously ignored as a possibility of disease (Barsky and Kleinman, 1983). This process confirms the self-validating / self-perpetuating cycle of symptom magnification and disease opinion. The various assumptions in previous research lend towards the confusion of meta-information. Specifically, there is a lot of research that contradicts or correlates with other research, as noted in the above paragraphs. Therefore, this research is justified by the ambiguity of previous research. However, it is important to note that conflicting information does not mean that previous research in this area is invalid, but it does mean that the concept of illness perception and symptoms has not been explored to the fullest extent. Ranjith & Mohan (2006) explicates that the increasing migration and globalization, illness presentations are no longer confined to the boundaries of geography as well as overarching models integrating known biological and psychological factors and sociocultural factors. The role of culture in shaping illness behavior, lending itself to use in clinical settings. Helping to explain certain unique illness attributions and help-seeking patterns. A theoretical model proposed by Mumford (1989) provides an explanation how the concept of culture does its role in shaping the progression or evolution of somatic symptoms. The model explained the three levels wherein culture may be responsible for the formation of the somatic symptoms: a) Symptom expression from language and idiom used; b) Health and disease concepts that are known to cause influence symptom interpretations and; c) A culturally sanctioned illness behavior which has been considered important in symptom presentation. In the same line, Potts and Bass (1995) indicate that locating symptom origins in psychological processes within the patient tends to credit responsibility, including moral blame that patients may understandably resist. Kleinman & Becker (1998) also stated that many systems of medicine consider sociosomatic theories recognizing origins of somatic distress may lie within the social context. Therefore, the significance of this study extends beyond the understanding of causation and perception, but will develop evidence towards the behaviors that people perceive as causing illness. The cultural perception of the language used in the methodology-stressed, injured, overworked, whiplash---will lead towards supporting evidence that perception is not wholly based on the cause and effect factors of an illness, but the cultural and cognitive perceptions of that illness. Health Locus of Control (HLOC) The Health Locus of Control is used in this research as a dependant variable. The assumption is made that HLOC will have an impact on the attribution of cause. The Health Locus of Control (HLOC) examines an individual's personal attribution of health. Those with internal LOC believe that illness is under their personal control while individuals with external LOC feel that the environment dominates their health. The Multidimensional Health Locus of Control Scale (MHLC) a study examined the connection between high school students' LOC and their grade-point averages, extracurricular activity membership, amount of studying time, alcohol use, seatbelt use and tobacco use (Harvey & Thomas, 2004). The theory of LOC was derived from Rotter's Social Learning Theory of 1954 (Rotter, 1982). Rotter published his Locus of Control Scale to measure generalized perceptions of individuals toward internal or external LOC. Researchers first detected the use of LOC in the medical community in observations of recently diagnosed diabetics and medical professionals (Kaplan, Maides, Wallston & Wallston, 1976). The medical staffers were attempting to get the patients and their families to develop an internal LOC that would better the chances of controlling their illness and improve overall quality of life (Kaplan, Maides, Wallston & Wallston, 1976). Multidimensional HLOC: Wallston et al (1978) is: Internality: the extent to which LoC for health is internal (e.g. "If I become sick, I have the power to make myself well again"); Chance: the belief that chance or external factors are affecting the outcome of health problems (e.g. "Often I feel sick no matter what I do"); Powerful Others: the belief in the control of powerful others over our health (e.g. "Following doctor's orders is the best way for me to stay healthy"). Locus of Control (LOC) also refers to personal belief that the events, such as illness, are either a result of personal control and effort, or outside forces such as fate and luck. Perception of positive and negative events as being consequences of one's own actions and thereby under one's own personal control is known as internal LOC. In contrast, external LOC refers to the perception of positive or negative events being unrelated to one's own behavior and thereby beyond personal control. (Locus of Control and Cardiovascular Health, 2004). A Multidimensional Health Locus of Control Scale (MHLC) was developed in 1976 (Wallston, Wallston, & DeVellis, 1978). Mackey (2002) noted a great deal of research linked internal LOC to positive health beliefs and behaviors. According to the author, individuals with an internal LOC were more likely to seek health-related knowledge, successfully stop smoking, maintain better weight control, adhere to physician's prescriptions, use birth control effectively, seek preventative vaccines, use a seatbelt regularly, and practice proper dental hygiene. MacArthur and MacArthur (1999) added that a sense of control such as that experienced by internals led to improved emotional and mental health, reduced risk of heart disease, better self-rated health status, and lower morbidity. Therefore, it can be assumed that if: perception of cause effects behavior towards an illness, and HLOC affects the patient's overall ability to control and react to an illness, behavior and reaction towards an illness is directed by HLOC and attribution. Both concepts are then important to developing a holistic view of patient perceptions that leads to understanding behavior towards illness and cures. Hypothesis People, as noted above, have different perceptions of illness causation depending on their cognitive and cultural perception of that illness. The hypothesis seeks to answer two questions: (1) Does attribution of cause of the chronic illness depend on the acute illness context and label (2) Does attribution of cause depend on health locus of control The hypothesis will be examined using the following variables: Independent variables: the 6 different scenarios used being the 3 different illness context presented either with or without a label. The three illnesses are described in the following chapter. Dependent variables: 4 version of the causal section of the IPQ-R used for attribution of cause. The Multidimensional Health Locus of Control. References Bach, M. & Bach D., (1996). Alexithymia in somatoform disorder and somatic disease: A comparative study. Psychotherapy & Psychosomatics, 65, 150-152 Barsky, A, J., & Borus, J, F. (1999). Functional Somatic Syndromes. Annals of Internal Medicine, 130, 910-921. Barsky, A.J. & Borus, J.F. (1999). Functional Somatic Syndromes. Annals of Internal Medicine, 130, 910-921 Barsky, A.J. & Klerman, G.L. (1983). Overview: Hypochondriasis, bodily complaints and somatic styles. American Journal of Psychiatry, 140, 273-283 Baumann L J, Leventhal H 1985 "I can tell when my blood pressure is up, can't I" Health Psychology 4: 203-18 Becker, A.E & Kleinman, A. (2000). Anthropology and Psychiatry. In B.J. Saddock & V.A. Sadock (Eds.) Comprehensive textbook of psychiatry (pp.463-476). Philadelphia: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Behere, P.B. & Natraj, G.S. (1984). Dhat Syndrome: The phenomenology of a culture-bound sex neurosis of the orient. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 26, 76-78 Bhatia, M.S. (1999).An analysis of 60 cases of culture-bound syndromes. Indian Journal of Medical Sciences, 53, 149-152 Bhugra, D. & Buchanan, A. (1989). Impotence in the ancient Indian texts. Sexual and Marital Threapy, 4, 87-92 Bishop G D 1991 Understanding the understanding of illness: Lay disease representations. In: Skelton J A, Croyle R T (eds.) Mental Representations in Health and Illness (pp. 32-59). Springer-Verlag, New York NY Bishop G D, Converse S A 1986 Illness representations: A prototype approach. Health Psychology 5: 95-114 Cameron L, Leventhal E A, Leventhal H 1995 Seeking medical care in response to symptoms and life stress. Psychosomatic Medicine 57: 1-11 Cohen, Louis and Manion, Lawrence (1994), Research Methods in Education 4e, Routledge Collins, J. and Hussey, R. (2003) Business Research, 2nd Edition. Hampshire: Palgrave Macmillan. Creswell, J.W. (2003). Research design. Qualitative, quantitative and mixed methods approaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. Emmerova, Milada; Jirava, Frantiek (2004) Is Gulf War Syndrome Really a Mystery Medicine, Conflict & Survival, Jul2004, Vol. 20 Issue 3, p209-217, 9p Farrar, D.J., Locke, S.E. and Kantrowitz, F.G, (1995) 'Chronic fatigue syndrome 1: Etiology and pathogenesis'. Behavioural Medicine 21, 5- 16 Ferguson, E., & Cassaday, H. J. (1999). The Gulf War and illness by association. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 459-475. Garcia-Campayo, J. Lobo, A., Perez-Echevarria, M.J.& Campos, R. (1998).Three forms of somatization presenting in primary care settings in Spain. Journal of Nervous Mental Disorders, 186, 554-60 Ghauri, Pervez N., Grnhaug, Kjell (2002). Research Methods in Business Studies: A Practical Guide. Pearson: London Giorgi, A and Giorgi, B (2003) Phenomenology. In J A Smith (ed.) Qualitative psychology: a practical guide to research methods. London: Sage. Gordon, N. F. (1993). Chronic Fatigue Your Complete Exercise Guide. Leeds: Human Kinetics. Greiffenstein, Manfred F.; Baker, W. John. (2006) Miller was (mostly) right: Head injury severity inversely related to simulation. Legal & Criminological Psychology, Feb2006, Vol. 11 Issue 1, p131-145, 15p Groleau, D. & Kirmayer, L.J. (2004). Sociosomatic theory in Vietnamese immigrants narratives of distress. Anthropol Medical Journal, 11, 117-33 Gulf War Veterans: A Naturalistic Inquiry 2 Years After the Gulf War. Journal of Traumatic Stress, Jan98, Vol. 11 Issue 1, p165-171, 7p Hampson S E 1997 Illness representations and the self-management of diabetes. In: Petrie K J, Weinman J A (eds.) Perceptions of Health and Illness (pp. 323-47) Harvey, B. & Thomas, J. (2004). Examination Of Current Research: Locus Of Control, Self-Monitoring, Student Responsibility, And Academic Motivation, http://www.emporia.edu/tec/600dps3.htm Ikemi, Y. & Ikemi, A. (1986). An oriental point of view in psychosomatic medicine. Psychotherapy & Psychosomatics, 45, 118-126 Ikemi, Y. (1990). Psychosomatic Medicine as a core of holistic medicine.Shinshin-Igaku, 30, 251-260 Jason , L.A., King C.P., Richman, J.A. et al (1999) ' US case Definition of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome: Diagnostic and Theoretical issues' , Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, Vol 5 No 3/4 Haworth Press Binghampton,:3-33 Jason, L.A., King, C.P., Taylor, R. and Kennedy, C. (2000) 'Defining CFSyndrome: Methodological Challenges', Journal of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome, Vol 7 (3): 17-32 Kingham, Peter; Nicolaou, Andreas; Southwick, S. M.(1998) Anniversary Reactions in Kirmayer, L.J., Groleau,D, Looper, K.J. & Dao, M.D. (2004). Explaining Medically Unexplainable Symptoms. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 49, 10, 663 Kleinman, A. & Becker, A.E, (1998). Culture & Somatization: Clinical, epidemiological and ethnographic perspectives, Psychosomatic medicine, 60, 420-430 Komaroff, A.L. (2000) 'The biology of chronic fatigue syndrome' The American Journal of Medicine, Vol 108 (2): 169-171 Lau R R, Hartman K A 1983 Commonsense representations of common illnesses. Health Psychology 2: 167-85 Leventhal E, Crouch 1997 In Petrie K J, Weinman, J A (eds.) Perceptions of Health and Illness. Harwood Academic, Amsterdam Leventhal H, Meyer D, Nerenz D 1980 The common sense representation of illness danger. In: Rachman S (ed.) Contributions to Medical Psychology (Vol. 2). Pergamon Press, New York Leventhal H, Nerenz D R, Steele D J 1984 Illness representations and coping with health threats. In: Baum A, Singer J (eds.) A Handbook of Psychology and Health (Vol. 4, pp. 219-252). Erlbaum, Hillsdale NJ Lipowski, Z.J., (1988). 'Somatization: The concept and its clinical application' American Journal of Psychiatry 145, 1358-1368 Locus of control and Cardiovascular health (2004) Job Stress At Work. Work Health Organization http://www.workhealth.org/index.html MacArthur, C. & MacArthus, J. (1999). Personal control Research Network on Socioeconomic Status and Health. Research Network on Socioeconomic Status and Health http://www.macses.ucsf.edu/Research/Psychosocial/notebook/control.html Mackey, A. (2002). Power, Pessimism & Prevention: The Impact Of Locus Of Control On Physical Health, http://www.units.muohio.edu/psybersite/control/health.shtml Mal'occhiu, M.S. (1997). Ambiguity, evil eye and the language of distress. London: University of Toronto Press Manu, R. (1998). Functional Somatic Syndromes: Etiology, diagnosis and treatment. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press Matthews K A 1982 Psychological perspectives on the Type A behavior pattern. Psychological Bulletin 91: 293-323. Maxwell, J. A. Qualitative Research Design: An Interactive Approach. Thousand Oaks: Sage Publications, 1996. Mayou, R., & Radanov, B.R. (1996). Whiplash Neck Injury. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 40(5), 461-474. McCully, K.K., Sisto, S.A., and Natelson, B.H. (1996). 'Use of exercise for treatment of chronic fatigue syndrome'. Sports Medicine 21(1) 35-48 McDonough, J. and McDonough, S. (1997), Research methods for English language Mechanic D 1972 Social psychologic factors affecting the presentation of bodily complaints. New England Journal of Medicine 286: 1132-39 Meyer D, Leventhal H, Gutmann M 1985 Common-sense models of illness: The example of hypertension. Health Psychology 4: 115-35 Miller H, Cartlidge N. (1972) Simulation And Malingering After Injuries To The Brain And Spinal Cord. Lancet Medical Journal. 1972 Mar 11;1(7750):580-585. Moss-Morris, R., Weinman, J., Petrie, K. J., Horne, R., Cameron, L.D., & Buick, D. (2002). The Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R). Psychology and Health. 17, 1-16. Moss-Morris, R., Weinman, J., Petrie, K. J., Horne, R., Cameron, L.D., & Buick, D. (2002). The Revised Illness Perception Questionnaire (IPQ-R). Retrieved 01 August 2005, from The Illness Perception Questionnaire Website: http://www.uib.no/ipq/ Mumford, D.B. (1989). Somatic Sensations and psychological distress among students in Britain and Pakistan. Social Psychiatry & Psychiatric Epidemology, 24, 321-326 Nebel, K.; Stude, P.; Ldecke, C.; Wiese, H.; Diener, H.-C.; Keidel, M.. Cephalalgia, (2005) Prospective PC-interactive pressure algesimetry of post-traumatic neck pain after whiplash injury. Mar2005, Vol. 25 Issue 3, p205-213, 9p Obyesekere, G. (1977). The theory and practice of psychological medicine in the Ayurvedic tradition. Culture Medicine & Psychiatry, 1, 155-181 Oliver, Richard L. 1980. A cognitive model of the antecedents and consequences of satisfaction decisions. J. Marketing Res. 42 (November) 460-469 Pennebaker J W 1982 The Psychology of Physical Symptoms. Springer-Verlag, New York Potts, S. & Bass, C. (1995). Psychological morbidity in patients with chest pain and normal or near-normal coronary arteries: a long-term follow-up study. Psychology Medical Journal, 24, 339-347 Ranjit, G. & Mohan, R. (2006). Dhat Syndrome as a Functional Somatic Syndrome: Developing a Sociosomatic Model. Psychiatry, 69, 2, 142 Rosof, B. M., & Hernandez, L. M. (2001). Gulf War Veterans: Treating Symptoms and Syndromes. Washington, DC, USA: National Academies Press. Retrieved 10 August 2005, from University of Portsmouth, Ebrary Web site: http://site.ebrary.com/lib/portsmouth/TopchannelName=portsmouth&cpage=1&docID=10038701&f00=text&frm=smp.x&hitsPerPage=10&layout=document&p00=gulf+war+syndrome&sortBy=score&sortOrder=desc Rotter, J.B. (1982). The Development and Application of Social Learning Theory. New York: Praeger. Safer M A, Tharps Q J, Jackson T C, Leventhal H 1979 Determinants of three stages of delay in seeking care at a medical care clinic. Medical Care 17: 11-29 Salovey P, Birnbaum D 1989 Influence on mood on health-relevant cognitions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57: 539-51 Saunders, M., Lewis, P. and Thornhill, D. (2003), Research Methods for Business Students, 3rd Edition, London: FT Prentice Hall. Schachter S, Singer J E 1962 Cognitive, social and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review 69: 379-99 Schrader, Harald; Obelieniene, Diana. Lancet (1996) Natural evolution of late whiplash syndrome outside the medicolegal context, 05/04/96, Vol. 347 Issue 9010, p1207, 5p, 7 charts; Shifneos, P.E. (1973). The prevalence of alexithymic characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy & Psychosomatics, 22,255-262 Shilling, C. (1993) The body and social theory, Sage, London Shipko, S. (1982). Alexithymia & Somatization. Psychotherapy & Psychosomatics, 37, 193-201 Shorter, E. (1992). From Paralysis to fatigue: a history of psychosomatic illness in the modern era. New York: Macmillan Siegel K, Gorey E 1997 HIV infected women: Barriers to AZT use. Social Science and Medicine 45: 15-22 Soetekouw; de Vries; van Bergen; Galama; Keyser; Bleijenberg; van der Meer; Soetekouw. (2000) Somatic hypotheses of war syndromes. European Journal of Clinical Investigation, Jul2000, Vol. 30 Issue 7, p630-641, 12p, 3 charts, 1 diagram Tarres, M.V. (1999). Tackling Functional Somatic Syndromes with confidence. The Lancet, 354, 874 Turner, B.S. with Sampson, C.(1995) Medical power and social knowledge, London, Sage Valins S 1966 Cognitive effects of false heart rate feedback. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 4: 400-08 Wallston, B., Wallston, K., Kaplan, G. & Maides, S. (1976). Development and Validation of the Health Locus of Control (HLC) Scales. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. Vol. 44 Issue 4. Wallston, K. A., Wallston, B. S., & DeVillis, R. (1978). Development of the multidimensional health locus of control (MHLC) scales. Health Education Monographs, 6, 161-170. In J. Weinman, S. Wright & M. Johnston, Measures in Health Psychology: A User's Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs. (P 1-11). Berskshire: The NFER-NELSON Publishing Company Ltd. Ware, N.C. (1992) 'Suffering and the social construction of illness: the de-legitimation of illness experience in chronic fatigue syndrome'. Medical Anthropological Quarterly 6(4):347-61 Weiner, Bernard (1998) Taking Too Many Chances With Chance. Psychological Inquiry, 1998, Vol. 9 Issue 2, p113, 3p Weiner, H. (1992). Perturbing the organism: the biology of stressful experience. Chicago Illinois: University of Chicago Press Weinman, J., Wright, S., & Johnston, M. Measures in Health Psychology: A User's Portfolio. Causal and Control Beliefs. (P 1-11). Berskshire: The NFER-NELSON Publishing Company Ltd. Weinstein N D 1980 Unrealistic optimism about future life events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 39: 806-20. Wessely, S (1995) 'The epidemiology of chronic fatigue syndrome' Epidemiological Review, 17:139-151 Wessley, S., Nimnuan, C. & Sharpe, M. (1998). Functional Somatic Syndrome: one or many Lancet, 354, 936-939 Wessley, S., Nimnuan, C., & Sharpe, M. (1999). Functional somatic syndromes: one or many Lancet, 354, 936-39. Wickramasekera, I.E. (1995). Clinical Behavioral Medicine. Some concepts and procedures. New York: Plenum Wise, T. (1999). Psychosomatics. ProQuest Psychology Journals, 447 Read More
Cite this document
  • APA
  • MLA
  • CHICAGO
(“Why do people term an illness to be physcial or psychological Is there Essay”, n.d.)
Why do people term an illness to be physcial or psychological Is there Essay. Retrieved from https://studentshare.org/miscellaneous/1529310-why-do-people-term-an-illness-to-be-physcial-or-psychological-is-there-a-relationship-between-public-causal-attributions-of-functional-somatic-syndromes-and-h
(Why Do People Term an Illness to Be Physcial or Psychological Is There Essay)
Why Do People Term an Illness to Be Physcial or Psychological Is There Essay. https://studentshare.org/miscellaneous/1529310-why-do-people-term-an-illness-to-be-physcial-or-psychological-is-there-a-relationship-between-public-causal-attributions-of-functional-somatic-syndromes-and-h.
“Why Do People Term an Illness to Be Physcial or Psychological Is There Essay”, n.d. https://studentshare.org/miscellaneous/1529310-why-do-people-term-an-illness-to-be-physcial-or-psychological-is-there-a-relationship-between-public-causal-attributions-of-functional-somatic-syndromes-and-h.
  • Cited: 0 times

CHECK THESE SAMPLES OF Perception of Cause and Behavior towards an Illness

A Business Plan: Developing a Nurse Practitioner Led Wellness and Recovery Center

According to the Treatment Advocacy Center (2012), a national nonprofit dedicated to eliminating barriers to the treatment of mental illness, the next several years will see virtually all patients currently under state care in Alabama forcibly released into the community.... This de-institutionalization of patients is bound to cause several problems in health care and it would take a long time to build replacement facilities for these patients across the state, which includes 16-bed regional homes and crisis centers....
11 Pages (2750 words) Dissertation

Community-based Day Treatment Centers

BUSINESS PLAN FOR A WELLNESS AND RECOVERY CENTER FOR DEINSTITUTIONALIZED INDIVIDUALS CHAPTER TWO REVIEW OF LITERATURE BY AUDRY GREEN GORMAN DOCTOR OF NURSING PRACTICE CLINICAL PROJECT CHAPTER 2 Review of Literature As long-time residents of a mental institution, being deinstitutionalized to go back to community living may be unnerving especially for the adults with serious mental illness.... Nursing Care Nursing care at the community level has shown much positive outcomes for patients with mental illness, providing them with a better quality of life....
14 Pages (3500 words) Dissertation

The Relationships of the Causes and Acceptance of Mental Illness and Behaviour

The paper "The Relationships of the Causes and Acceptance of Mental illness and Behaviour" attempted to examine the relationship shared by the extent to which behaviour is perceived as indicative of mental illness and the attribution of the same to physical and psycho-social causes.... The use of labels seems to be a greater reason for segregation than the illness itself, and this brings the whole procedure of labelling an individual's condition under question....
7 Pages (1750 words) Research Paper

Sociology of Health and Illness

The paper "Sociology of Health and illness" narrates change of status is the major causative agent to the breakdown of Smythe's family.... Rupert's situation can only be understood through highlighting factors anti-discriminatory practice and empowerment, psychological factors, health promotion and communication, multi-professional working, and Sociology of health and illness.... Rupert should be empowered to go beyond his feelings of disappointment and act towards making his life better....
5 Pages (1250 words) Essay

Behaviour and Cognitive Therapies According to Psychotherapy

The prevalence of mental illness among racial and ethnic minorities is generally similar to that for whites but disparities exist in access to, availability of, and quality of mental health services.... Generally, minority communities who are more likely to experience mental disorders than whites, have more stigmas regarding mental illness and are less likely to seek early treatment....
14 Pages (3500 words) Research Paper

Understanding Specific Needs in Health and Social Care

The index of understanding these concepts entails all perspectives that address the subject as relates to human behaviour regarding illness.... It is essential since it facilitates the interventions entailed in caring and understanding these special needs regarding disability, illness and behaviour.... In the paper 'Understanding Specific Needs in Health and Social Care' the author facilitates an understanding from a holistic perspective towards disability and care for the disabled persons....
16 Pages (4000 words) Essay

Cultural Perceptions Toward Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders

The paper "Cultural Perceptions Toward Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders" focuses on exploring how different cultures and different countries perceive autism and how this perception may impact the rehabilitation and acceptance of the autistic individuals.... ... ... ... Culture is also defined in terms of shared cognitions, shared standards of behaviors and assumed roles and self-definitions that members indulge in....
12 Pages (3000 words) Case Study

Mental Illnesses: Altered Perception

The paper 'Mental Disorders Prevalence in the Australian Population, Signs, and Symptoms of Altered Perception, Mental illness Nexus Physical Health, and Impacts' is a forceful a variant of case study on health sciences & medicine.... The paper 'Mental Disorders Prevalence in the Australian Population, Signs, and Symptoms of Altered Perception, Mental illness Nexus Physical Health, and Impacts' is a forceful a variant of case study on health sciences & medicine....
12 Pages (3000 words) Case Study
sponsored ads
We use cookies to create the best experience for you. Keep on browsing if you are OK with that, or find out how to manage cookies.
Contact Us